Methodology in Language Learning: Language Aptitude and Age
Does
language aptitude change with age either in a positive or in a negative way? On
the one hand, if language aptitude is indeed a trait, it should be relatively
stable. Intelligence, for example, has been found to be remarkably stable. The other side of the coin, however, is that age
is a central factor in an individual’s language learning capacity—as evidenced by
the vast amount of literature on the ‘critical period hypothesis’ addressing
age-related changes in SLA—and therefore it is not unreasonable to assume that
some of the age-related variation is mediated through aptitude changes that
occur over time.
In
sum, there are several findings in this study that provide some support nor the
argument that analytical language ability is more closely associated with
second language outcomes when intensive exposure to the language is first
experienced in adolescence. This relationship appears to hold, though not as
strongly, even when exposure takes place in an environment outside the second
language classroom.
One
of the most persistent issues in the L2 aptitude literature has been the relationship
between language aptitude and general intelligence. This is understandable: If
the predictive power of language aptitude is almost entirely due to the
commonalities it shares with intelligence, we would need to reconsider the
importance attached to the construct, whereas if we find that aptitude exerts
its influence above that of intelligence, that would confirm the validity of
the concept. Of course, we should realize that the whole issue is somewhat
artificial because past research has revealed that both intelligence and
language aptitude are composite constructs, subsuming a number of distinct
components. Therefore, it is likely that instead of a clear-cut relationship between
the two higher-order factors (i.e., ‘intelligence’ and ‘language aptitude’)
there is a complex pattern of interrelationships between their constituent
components: Some cognitive components of general (i.e., non-language-specific)
mental abilities will undoubtedly play a role in one’s language learning
capacity, whereas some others might be irrelevant. It all depends on how we
conceptualize the two constructs and what learning situation we are considering
(the situational dependence of language aptitude will be discussed in detail
later).
There
are a variety of reasons for which aptitude test scores can be used:
Ø Research:
An unambiguous area of employing aptitude tests is in research studies in which
scholars want to control for or further investigate cognitive ability factors.
Ø Selection:
The most obvious application of a language aptitude test is in selection
procedures and, indeed, most of the tests described earlier have been used as
selection devices in different contexts. The Defense Language Aptitude Battery,
for example, was developed and employed for the purpose of the selection of
army personnel for intensive language training programs. By doing so it was hoped
that the costs of the training and the amount of time it took could be reduced
and hopeless language learners could be screened out.
Ø Allocating
resources: By streaming language learners according to their aptitude scores,
program administrators can have a more precise understanding of the extent of
extra resources that the lower-aptitude groups might need to achieve the
required level of proficiency.
Ø Program
evaluation: By administering aptitude tests it may be possible to compare the
learners’ actual achievement with the achievement one might expect on the basis
of their L2 learning ability. This would allow for a more accurate evaluation
of the effectiveness of language teaching programs.
Ø Tailoring
instruction to the learners’ aptitude level: From an educational point of view
this might be the most interesting line of research. Several scholars have
suggested that aptitude tests can be used to identify the particular cognitive
strengths and learning style preferences of groups of learners, so that this
diagnostic information can be used to tailor the quality and quantity of
language instruction accordingly. Let us stop here for a moment and elaborate
on this issue.
Profiling
individual differences in cognitive abilities, and matching these profiles to
effective instructional options, such as types of pedagogic tasks,
interventionist ‘focus on form’ techniques, and more broadly defined learning
conditions, is a major aim of pedagogically oriented language aptitude research.
The
term language aptitude is becoming increasingly restricted to refer to composite
measures obtained by means of aptitude batteries, whereas scholars who focus on
specific cognitive abilities, such as working memory, tend to avoid using the
term. Contextually sensitive measures of language aptitude open up brand new
possibilities for integrating aptitude research into mainstream SLA studies,
and they also allow researchers to link cognitive abilities to instructed SLA
and classroom practice in a useful way.
©
University
of Oxford - post gradual studies 2009 'English Language Teaching'
Bibliography:
1) Blundell, Lesley and Stokes, Jackie, Task
listening, Cambridge University Press, 198r.
2) Gore, Lesley, Listening to Maggie, Longman,
1979.
3) McClintock, John and Stern, Borje, Let's
listen, Heinemann Educational Books, 1974.
4) Maley, Alan and Moulding, Sandra, Learning to
listen, CambridgeUniversity Press, 198 I.
5) Scott, Wendy, Are you listening?, Oxford
University Press, 1980.
6) Stokes, Jacqueline StClair, Elementary task
listening, CambridgeUniversity Press, 1984.
7) Underwood, Mary and Barr, Pauline, Listeners
(series), Oxford University Press, 1980.
8) Abbs, Brian and Jones, T., Cloudsongs,
Longman, 1977.
9) Abbs, Brian and York, N., Skyhigh, Longman,
1975.
10) Jones, Christopher, Back home,
Longman, 1980.
11) Kingsbury, Roy, and O'Shea, Patrick,
Seasons and people and other songs,Oxford University Press, 1979.
12) Wilson, Ken, Mister Monday and other
songs for the teaching of English,Longman, 197r.
13) Wilson, Ken and Morrow, Keith, Goodbye
rainbow, Longman, 1974.
14) Seidl, Jennifer and McMordie, W.,
English idioms and how to use them, Oxford University Press, 1978.
15) Wilson, F. P. (ed. ), Oxford
Dictionary of English Proverbs, Oxford University Press, 1970.
16) Brown, Gillian, Listening to spoken
English, Longman, 1977.
17) Brown, Gillian, 'Understanding spoken
language', TESOL Quarterly 12:2, 1978.
18) Brown, Gillian and Yule, George,
Teaching the spoken language, Cambridge University Press, 1983.
19) Byrne, Donn, 'Listening
comprehension', Teaching oral English, Longman, 1976.
20) Crystal, David and Davy, Derek,
Investigating English style, Longman, 1969:
21) Curfs, Emile, 'Listening deserves
better', Modern English Teacher 9:3, 1982.
22) Geddes, Marion, 'Listening', inK.
Johnson and K. Morrow (eds. ),
23) Communication in the classroom,
Longman, 1981. Geddes, Marion and White, Ron, 'The use of semi-scripted
simulated authentic speech and listening comprehension', Audio-visual Language
journal, 1978.
24) Littlewood, William, Communicative
language teaching, Cambridge University Press, 198 I.
25) Maley, Alan, 'The teaching of
listening comprehension skills', Modern English Teacher, 1978.
26) Porter, Don and Roberts, Jon,
'Authentic listening activities', English Language Teaching]ournal, 1981.
27) Richards, Jack C., 'Listening
comprehension', TESOL Quarterly, 1983.
28) Rivers, Wilga, 'Hearing and
comprehending', Teaching foreign language skills (revised edn.), University of
Chicago Press, 1980. Widdowson, Henry, Teaching language as communication,
Oxford University Press, 1978. The teaching of listening comprehension, British
Council, E.L. T. Documents Special, 1981
Komentarze
Prześlij komentarz